The Reliability of Human Disambiguation in Text Markup

paper
Authorship
  1. 1. Kevin J. Keen

    Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics - Case Western Reserve University

  2. 2. Paul A. Fortier

    Department of French, Spanish and Italian - University of Manitoba

Work text
This plain text was ingested for the purpose of full-text search, not to preserve original formatting or readability. For the most complete copy, refer to the original conference program.


The Reliability of Human Disambiguation in Text
Markup

Kevin
J.
Keen
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics Case Western Reserve University
kjkeen@darwin.cwru.edu

Paul
A.
Fortier
Department of French, Spanish, and Italian University of Manitoba
fortier@cc.umanitoba.ca

1999

University of Virginia

Charlottesville, VA

ACH/ALLC 1999

editor

encoder

Sara
A.
Schmidt

Semantic Fields
Studying semantic fields or literary themes in texts confronts researchers
with a paradox. A computer string search will produce a list of the
frequencies of words potentially related to the semantic field. But polysemy
implies an un-measurable difference between the potential allusion and the
real allusion. The semantic field of solitude is
important from sociological and psychological perspectives as an indication
of imperfect adaptation to one's milieu. It is also a frequently occurring
literary theme.
The only practical response is disambiguation by human informants. But the
reliability of such a process is a concern. Singh (1986) and Fokkema (1988)
discuss the matter in a theoretical and speculative mode without systematic
empirical evidence. Nothing bearing directly on the topic can be found in
recent issues of Computers and the Humanities or
Literary and Linguistic Computing.
The correctness of a given choice by an individual informant or rater is
generally unknowable. Usually, such a choice is a matter of degree and
judgment within the cultural context of a language. The consistency of
markup from one informant to another is a likely reasonable touchstone for
assessing the reliability of the disambiguation process.

Data
Nine 20th century novels were chosen for this analysis: Bernanos, Journal d'un curé de campagne, Camus, L'Étranger and La Chute, Céline,
Voyage au bout de la nuit, Gide, L'Immoraliste and La Porte
étroite, Mauriac, Le Noeud de Vipères,
Proust, La Fugitive, and Sartre, La Nausée.
French thesauri identify seventy words (in the sense of lemmata) related to
the concept of solitude. These strings were used to search the texts for
words potentially related to solitude in the ARTFL database. Words centered
in 60 characters of context were given to six informants with minimal
instructions: mark the words, which from a reading of the context, do not
evoke the concept of human solitude and go back to the ARTFL database for
more context in doubtful cases.
Table 1 summarizes the number of allusions to human solitude. Note that the
range of the number of counts of allusions to solitude varies widely
according to the text examined.
To assess the impact of providing a greater amount of context to informants,
300 characters of context from the original text were obtained using the
same set of strings as for 60 characters of context. Identical instructions
were given to six informants--two of whom participated in the first
project.
Table 2 summarizes the number of allusions to human solitude for a context of
300 characters. When a larger context is provided, the range of variability
in the results increases. On the other hand, the consistency of the raters
is slightly improved.

Analysis
The sample intraclass correlation coefficient, denoted as formula ICC(3,1) by
Shrout and Fleiss (1979), has been chosen as the measure of agreement among
the informants. For dichotomous responses, an intraclass correlation
coefficient of one-quarter for a sample consisting of two informants is to
be interpreted to reveal that two informants were in agreement for
twenty-five per cent of the observations after correcting for the
possibility that any agreements of the two informants were entirely due to
chance
The measure of reliability of a sum of responses from a random sample of a
set of a fixed number of raters drawn from a population of raters is known
as the Spearman-Brown prophecy after Spearman (1910) and Brown (1910).
Cronbach's alpha, due to Cronbach (1951), is an estimate of the
Spearman-Brown prophecy.
Both the Spearman-Brown prophecy and Cronbach's alpha are increasing
functions of the intraclass correlation--parameter and statistic,
respectively--and each asymptotically equal 100% in the limit as the number
of raters increases without bound despite the consideration that adding more
raters only increases the chances that everybody will not agree.
Statements made regarding a population based on a sample must be qualified by
a probability clause. The standard choice for a probability clause is such
that the statement be correct in the long run, if the statistical procedure
were to be done unboundedly many times, 95% of the time--or nineteen times
out of twenty. So a measure of the degree of difficulty associated with a
specific text in determining whether there is an allusion to solitude is
given by the minimum number of informants to achieve a value of 95% for
Cronbach's alpha nineteen times out of twenty.

Results
Hypothesis tests revealed statistically significant differences regarding
reliability among the different texts and among the two character-string
lengths. Further exploratory analysis ruled out a particular informant, a
particular part of speech, or a particular class of frequency of use as
being influential.
The 95% confidence intervals for the intraclass correlation coefficient are
given in Table 3 for both 60 character and 300 character groups surrounding
each type. Note that for each novel the confidence interval for rater
reliability is shifted to the left with the 300 character groups compared to
the 60 character groups. With more context, the estimate of reliability
becomes lower.
Note the lack of overlap for the 95% confidence intervals for the two sizes
of character groups for each of the novels by Bernanos, Céline, and Proust.
It is conjectured that as the sample size surrounding each type increases,
there is more opportunity for subjectivity based on personal opinion.
Table 4 presents the number of informants required to achieve 95% for
Cronbach's alpha nineteen times out of twenty for 60 and 300 character
contexts. For each novel, as the size of context increases, the number of
informants required to achieve 95% for Cronbach's alpha increases. Moreover,
the size of a jury to decide whether a word alludes to human solitude with
300 characters of context ought to be no fewer than 45 based on the novels
surveyed.
With respect to literary analysis, note that the novels of Camus have a
greater spread in jury size than those of Gide.

Conclusion
It appears that the use of informants for studying semantic fields, or
literary themes, is justifiable from a statistical perspective. However, the
large numbers of informants or jury members appears prohibitive.
Paradoxically, reliability appears to decrease as the number of characters
of content increases. Further studies are needed to determine whether
increases in reliability can be obtained by changing the focus from a word
to a sentence or longer passage.
These results suggest a high degree of subjectivity when a single individual
scores the semantic content of literary data. The meaning of individual
words in context is a matter of opinion, and cannot be taken as definitive
until a high degree of consensus among a large number of raters or
informants is achieved.

Table 1Scores for Human Solitude with 60 Characters of
Context

Texts

raw

p1

p2

s1

s2

s3

s4

BJC
260
68
89
51
44
100
91

CET
74
22
19
13
11
23
26

CCH
115
32
41
26
31
36
41

CVN
488
89
133
78
41
127
153

GIM
89
28
42
36
18
47
48

GPE
108
26
41
36
26
37
54

MNV
159
42
70
47
28
92
71

PFU
304
49
80
57
44
82
76

SNA
233
111
113
77
86
95
122

Table 2.Scores for Human Solitude with 300 Characters of
Context

Texts

raw

p3

p4

p5

s5

s6

s7

BJC
260
98
44
154
45
73
51

CET
74
23
11
48
6
14
10

CCH
115
37
34
61
23
30
26

CVN
488
153
64
199
53
85
60

GIM
89
42
29
49
13
25
22

GPE
108
36
29
49
13
25
22

MNV
159
80
32
113
28
41
43

PFU
304
68
53
126
49
69
44

SNA
233
114
96
151
67
70
79

Table 3.95% confidence interval: intraclass correlation
coefficient

Texts

60 Characters

300 characters

Lower

Upper

Lower

Upper

BJC
0.56
0.66
0.43
0.54

CET
0.48
0.68
0.29
0.51

CCH
0.68
0.79
0.54
0.69

CVN
0.51
0.59
0.42
0.50

GIM
0.40
0.59
0.28
0.48

GPE
0.43
0.60
0.33
0.50

MNV
0.47
0.60
0.33
0.50

PFU
0.63
0.71
0.49
0.59

SNA
0.59
0.69
0.48
0.59

Table 4.Number of informants required for a value of 95% or more
for Cronbach's alpha nineteen times out of twenty.

Texts

60 Characters

300 Characters

BJC
15
25

CET
19
43

CCH
9
16

CVN
18
26

GIM
28
45

GPE
25
37

MNV
21
25

PFU
12
19

SNA
14
21

Explanation of Symbols used in the Tables Texts: BJC: Bernanos, Journal d'un curé de campagne CET: Camus, L'Étranger CCH: Camus, La
Chute CVN: Céline, Voyage au bout de la
nuit GIM: Gide, L'Immoraliste
GPE: Gide, La Porte étroite MNV: Mauriac,
Le Noeud de Vipères PFU: Proust, La Fugitive SNA: Sartre, La
Nausée.
Informants: raw: raw solitude data as downloaded from the ARTFL
database s(1-7): French Literature Graduate Student p(1-5): French
Literature Professor

References

W.
Brown

Some experimental results in the correlation of mental
abilities

British Journal of Psychology

3

296-322
1910

Georges
Bernanos

Journal d'un curé de campagne
Oeuvres Romanesques
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

Albert
Béguin

Paris
Gallimard
1935
1961

Albert
Camus

L'Étranger
Théâtre, Récits, Nouvelles
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

Roger
Quilliot

Paris
Gallimard
1945
1962

Albert
Camus

La Chute
Théâtre, Récits, Nouvelles
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

Roger
Quilliot

Paris
Gallimard
1956
1962

L.-F.
Céline

Voyage au bout de la nuit
Romans
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

H.
Mondor

Paris
Gallimard
1932
1962

J.
Cohen

A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales

Educational and Psychological Measurement

20

37-46
1960

L.
J.
Cronbach

Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of
tests

Psychometrika

16

297-334
1951

Douwe
Fokkema

On the reliability of literary studies

Poetics Today

9

529-543
1988

André
Gide

L'Immoraliste
Romans, Récits, Soties, Oeuvres lyriques
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

Y.
Davet

J.-J.
Thierry

Paris
Gallimard
1902
1958

André
Gide

Porte étroite
Romans, Récits, Soties, Oeuvres lyriques
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

Y.
Davet

J.-J.
Thierry

Paris
Gallimard
1909
1958

François
Mauriac

Le Noeud de Vipères

Paris
Grasset
1932

Marcel
Proust

La Fugitive
À la recherche du temps perdu
Bibliothèque de la Pleiade

Pierre
Clarac

André
Ferré

3 vol.
Pars
Gallimard
1925
1954

Jean-Paul
Satre

La Nausée
Oeuvres Romanesques
Bibliothèque de la Pléiade

Michel
Contat

Michel
Rybalka

Paris
Gallimard
1938
1981

P.
E.
Shrout

J.
L.
Fleiss

Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater
reliability

Psychological Bulletin

86

420-428
1979

Gurbhagat
Singh

The identity of the literary text: Problems and
reliability of reader response: An inquiry into theoretical
positions

The Literary Criterion

21
4
49-57
1986

C.
Spearman

Correlation calculated with faulty data

British Journal of Psychology

3

271-295
1910

If this content appears in violation of your intellectual property rights, or you see errors or omissions, please reach out to Scott B. Weingart to discuss removing or amending the materials.

Conference Info

In review

ACH/ALLC / ACH/ICCH / ALLC/EADH - 1999

Hosted at University of Virginia

Charlottesville, Virginia, United States

June 9, 1999 - June 13, 1999

102 works by 157 authors indexed

Series: ACH/ICCH (19), ALLC/EADH (26), ACH/ALLC (11)

Organizers: ACH, ALLC

Tags
  • Keywords: None
  • Language: English
  • Topics: None